Author: admin

  • Water ‘Ponzi’ That Burned Jefferies Had Something for Everyone, Until It Didn’t

    Water ‘Ponzi’ That Burned Jefferies Had Something for Everyone, Until It Didn’t

    WaterStation vending machines stored at a warehouse, from court filings with the US Federal Bankruptcy Court.

    The pitch went like this: Good, safe drinking water has become such a scarce resource that Americans will pay to fill up jugs — 30 or 40 cents to the gallon — at dispensers all across the country.

    Most Read from Bloomberg

    Hundreds of investors bought thousands of units, believing in the vision laid out by Ryan Wear, founder of a startup called WaterStation Management. They plunked down $8,500 for each vending machine and then waited for the dispensers to throw off a steady stream of cash. Among those lured in was a product manager in Oregon in 2021 and then, several months later, a dentist in Illinois. Each bought dozens of water dispensers, which Wear’s team would install and operate.

    What they didn’t know is that at least one of those Hylyte-branded machines wound up being sold to both of them: serial number 101962, wedged in between a liquor store and a yoga studio in a strip mall in the Los Angeles suburb of Torrance, according to court records. To make matters worse, a machine with that same serial number was also pledged as collateral to back WaterStation bonds that were sold in April 2022 to the investment bank Jefferies Financial Group Inc.

    Serial number 101962, faded and rusted in pictures submitted to court, is now gone from that strip mall, WaterStation is in bankruptcy, and Wear, 49, is the target of legal action by federal prosecutors, the Securities and Exchange Commission, a state banking regulator, Jefferies and scores of small-time investors, all of whom claim the company’s business was largely an illusion.

    A photo of the Hylyte dispenser in Torrance, California, filed in federal bankruptcy court. Source: US Federal Bankruptcy Court
    A photo of the Hylyte dispenser in Torrance, California, filed in federal bankruptcy court. Source: US Federal Bankruptcy Court

    “This case involves a massive Ponzi scheme,” Assistant U.S. Attorney Justin Rodriguez told a federal judge in Manhattan Wednesday after Wear pleaded not guilty to criminal charges. A few feet away sat Jordan Chirico, a former Jefferies fund manager who prosecutors allege also committed fraud, directing his fund to purchase more WaterStation bonds after Wear admitted many water machines didn’t exist. He, too, pleaded not guilty. Lawyers for both declined to comment.

    Voluminous legal filings describe a business that drew in military veterans, stock traders, pharmacists, salespeople and retirees by leveraging growing fears about contaminated tap water and microplastics and offering a lucrative solution that’d churn out annual returns as high as 20%, year after year. That desire to make easy money, coupled with clever marketing and alleged diligence lapses, kept the sputtering business going until the money finally stopped flowing around June 2023, according to court documents.

    “These schemes never arise in a vacuum,” said John Bender, a lawyer who is representing a committee of WaterStation creditors. “The tragic consequences of the WaterStation affair could have been avoided had it not been for a cabal of insiders and institutions that prioritized their greed over doing the right thing even if it meant devastating the lives of a lot of people.”

    Earlier this month, the committee asked a judge overseeing the proceedings to rule that Wear’s business meets the legal definition of a Ponzi scheme — a type of operation that uses new money to pay returns of existing investors or other creditors, with promoters usually promising high returns for little risk. If granted, franchisees would likely get tax relief and advisers would have an easier time clawing back funds from entities that profited off WaterStation’s business, which “will lay the groundwork for future recoveries on behalf of victims of this Ponzi,” according to the committee’s filing.”

    Machine Mismatch

    Formed in 2016 in Everett, Washington, WaterStation Management sold upwards of 21,000 machines and raised more than $380 million over the course of some seven years. Growth was fueled by a handful of banks that issued loans backed by the US Small Business Administration, as well as about $100 million in bonds bought by a fund run by Jefferies. Wear claimed in a 2023 deposition that his business had more than 500 employees.

    In actuality, Wear’s firm only deployed roughly 2,100 machines, many likely never existed at all and most of the money WaterStation raised paid other costs or payments to existing franchisees, according to court records. Many dispensers that do exist were sold to multiple buyers — like the one that was in Torrance — or were promised as Jefferies’ collateral. Serial numbers on other machines don’t correspond with addresses investors were given by the company, court papers indicate. Determining who owns each machine and who bears responsibility for the alleged scheme is being fought over in federal court.

    To locate machines that were sold more than once, Bloomberg News analyzed thousands of serial numbers submitted in court by creditors with claims to WaterStation machines and more than a dozen investor lawsuits that have piled up. Restructuring advisers say in court papers that more than 10,000 water machines were sold to multiple purchasers.

    Falling Behind

    WaterStation had trouble paying franchisees for years before monthly payments stopped completely two years ago, according to Becky Yang O’Malley, a GlassRatner Advisory Services managing director and certified fraud examiner retained by a committee representing WaterStation franchisees and other creditors. Franchisees have sued WaterStation and Wear, who along with Chirico, was sued by the Jefferies fund, while banks have sued franchisees who have fallen behind on business loans, according to court records. Jefferies has also sued one lender, First Fed Bank, alleging it helped keep WaterStation afloat after becoming aware of the alleged fraud in order to prioritize repayment of debt it was owed.

    Wear in a sworn statement in April 2024 said franchisees’ allegations that machines don’t exist were untrue and their claims of fraud “are baseless, inflammatory and false.” Although WaterStation had occasionally experienced cash-flow issues, the business was legitimate and profits derived from water machines “were historically paid to plaintiffs,” Wear said at the time.

    A First Fed spokesman said the bank isn’t able to comment on specific aspects of Jefferies’ lawsuit “as this is an ongoing legal matter,” but that the lender did nothing wrong. The bank will be submitting a formal response to Jefferies’ complaint next month, “which will provide additional clarity at that time,” he said.

    ‘Financially Devastated’

    Chirico, 41, has also denied wrongdoing. His lawyer has said Chirico is also a victim of the WaterStation fraud and that Jefferies has “tried to scapegoat our client for an alleged scheme that deceived him along with hundreds of other investors and major institutions.”

    Jefferies’ 352 Capital fund, once managed by Chirico, filed a civil lawsuit against Chirico in New York state court after a federal judge in May dismissed an earlier complaint. The bond transactions and their risks “were no secret” to the firm and other institutions, Chirico’s lawyers said in a motion to dismiss the latest lawsuit. Chirico sought to protect the fund by removing Wear as manager and attempted “to stabilize the collateral so the possibility of a restructuring or refinancing could be explored,” according to his Aug. 14 motion.

    Restructuring advisers face a daunting task of trying to return money to franchisees who face substantial losses after Wear’s businesses went bankrupt last year. The situation is worse for those who took out loans to buy machines because even though the business was an alleged fraud, franchisees are still responsible for the debt and certain banks have sued borrowers who have fallen behind on payments. Some investors contend banks that partnered with Wear’s business should have uncovered the alleged scheme earlier because they had access to machine lists with duplicate serial numbers.

    “My family has been financially devastated by the WaterStation scheme,” one Indiana franchisee noted in a sworn statement. He said he spent $3.3 million on machines and took out loans from two banks to fund his investment, pushing his monthly loan payments to $35,000. He said WaterStation’s assurances that it would buy back machines and that the financing was “SBA-approved” made him believe the business was more profitable and secure than it actually was.

    Bank Loans

    WaterStation was listed on the SBA’s database of franchises eligible for agency-approved loans starting in 2018. It gained momentum two years later, when Wear hired former bank-loan officer Kevin Nooney to help forge ties with banks and build a financing program to boost machine sales. The arrangement brought in new investor cash as the pandemic triggered a plunge in interest rates that motivated Americans to pile into a raft of alternative investments during lockdowns.

    First Fed and fellow regional lenders Unibank and Celtic Bank were among the institutions that provided the most financing to investors, according to papers filed by a committee representing WaterStation creditors. Nooney said in a 2024 court filing that one of his former colleagues knew First Fed’s vice president of commercial lending, and that he also had “long-standing personal relationships” with Unibank’s former chief credit officer and a former loan officer.

    Unibank and Celtic participate in the SBA’s preferred lending program which lets private banks administer SBA-backed loans with minimal agency review. Preferred lenders approved 28,875 SBA loans worth nearly $30 billion in fiscal 2021, roughly 55% of all loans approved in the SBA’s flagship lending program, according to a 2022 congressional report.

    Unibank and Celtic didn’t respond to requests for comment.

    From the start, though, the machines that Wear’s business was built on never made enough money to pay investors or cover WaterStation’s other costs. Instead, Wear relied on investors’ money and other loans to pay returns he promised franchisees “and to perpetuate the illusion of a legitimate business,” according to Yang O’Malley’s report.

    As new money rolled in, people who already purchased machines got payouts they thought were their cut of the money generated from the vending business, according to court documents. WaterStation paid out $31.5 million in investor returns in 2021, about double what it paid in 2020, and more than $44 million in 2022, according to Yang O’Malley’s report.

    But cracks were already forming as soon as August 2021, when Nooney learned that WaterStation purchases could constitute a security, according to a complaint brought by Washington’s banking regulator in May. The company responded by altering how it pitched the opportunity and paid returns, and these changes had the effect of curtailing new purchases, the complaint said. A lawyer for Nooney didn’t return messages seeking comment.

    There was another problem with Wear’s business. The company pitched its machines as a way to make passive income, even though SBA rules say the loans WaterStation benefited from can only be used to fund actively managed franchises, according to the complaint. The state regulator also said WaterStation exploited the SBA’s preferred lender program.

    The SBA was “left in the dark” and relied on lenders to verify that funds for the WaterStation loans were being used for approved purposes, Washington authorities said.

    Enter Jefferies

    In need of fresh capital, Wear turned to the bond market. In 2022, a Jefferies hedge fund called 352 Capital purchased roughly $100 million in WaterStation bonds earmarked for machine purchases. The fund was run by Chirico, who had bought hundreds of machines prior to joining 352 Capital as portfolio manager, according to federal prosecutors. Chirico didn’t fully disclose to Jefferies his personal stake in WaterStation, according to the indictment, which he disputes.

    The bonds have spawned a separate Jefferies lawsuit against First Fed, which the firm claims became aware in the summer of 2022 that many machines didn’t exist. The lender, a unit of First Northwest Bancorp, had serial numbers for machines purchased with loans it gave franchisees, as well as machines WaterStation claimed ownership of that served as collateral for the bonds, “and hundreds of machines appeared on both lists,” according to Jefferies’ suit.

    First Fed has denied wrongdoing and last year sought a receiver to take over Wear’s business. In a July bankruptcy settlement, the bank also agreed to pay $2.87 million to creditors and make additional payments and concessions to benefit franchisees, according to court papers.

    First Fed in a statement this month said the bankruptcy settlement will benefit creditors because the bank released claims against WaterStation as well as liens on properties owned by an affiliate company. Proceeds from those assets will “become available for ratable distribution” to creditors, the bank said.

    ‘Going to Jail’

    As for Chirico, prosecutors allege he had “learned of serious issues at WaterStation” by the summer of 2023. Then, in a phone call the following January, Wear admitted that thousands of machines supporting the bonds didn’t exist. But instead of telling Jefferies, Chirico allegedly directed the fund to purchase more WaterStation bonds which Wear partly used to repay a debt to Chirico, according to the indictment.

    At Wednesday’s arraignment, Rodriguez, the prosecutor, told a federal judge that a “lengthy recorded phone call” is among the evidence law enforcement collected along with messages from Wear’s business email account. An investor who was also on the recorded phone call told Wear this was the “largest franchise fraud case in the history of the United States” and that he was “going to jail,” according to the indictments.

    As the legal process plays out, borrowers are still responsible for SBA loans even if they are victims of an alleged scam, said Paul Midzak, a lawyer who advises small business owners. However, borrowers like the WaterStation franchisees can raise the alleged fraud as a defense against their loans and challenge lenders in court, Midzak said. The SBA, meanwhile, can be slow in responding to borrowers and working with the agency can be “like dealing with a woolly mammoth,” he said.

    An SBA spokesman directed Bloomberg News to the Department of Justice. The DOJ in its press release announcing the criminal charges this month said that the SBA’s Office of Inspector General was among the federal agencies that assisted law enforcement in the criminal investigation.

    Outside court proceedings, the physical markers that remain of Wear’s business include the water dispensers that actually were deployed in places like Pahrump, Nevada, where a Hylyte machine sits near the Lakeside Casino & RV Park, serving water to travelers who stop at the casino for a $12 plate of steak and eggs before heading east to Las Vegas or west to Death Valley.

    Another 4,000-plus machines are sitting idle in two dozen abandoned warehouses stretching from Everett to Missoula, Montana, and Fort Meyers, Florida. Liquidation firm TAGeX Brands was hired to inventory machines. It discovered that people have broken into the warehouses to harvest copper wiring from the walls and that some packaged food left inside has attracted rats, according to court papers.

    The facilities “stand out as among the most disorganized warehouses I have encountered in my 38-year career,” TAGeX Chief Executive Officer Neal Sherman said in an August court filing. The machines lack insulation and pipes inside dispensers left in cooler climates often burst, making them impossible to sell.

    “The water machines were in poor condition,” Sherman said, “and were poorly made.”

    –With assistance from Nicola M White and Katanga Johnson.

    Most Read from Bloomberg Businessweek

    ©2025 Bloomberg L.P.

    Continue Reading

  • Oil prices steady amid Russia Ukraine peace uncertainty and supply dynamics – Invezz

    1. Oil prices steady amid Russia Ukraine peace uncertainty and supply dynamics  Invezz
    2. Oil prices set to end losing streak as Ukraine peace process stalls  Reuters
    3. Dollar Weakness and Strong Equity Markets Support Crude Oil Prices  inkl
    4. Inventory Drop Boosts WTI, Rate Cut Uncertainty Holds Markets  FOREX.com
    5. WTI Crude Oil Rallies Above 100-Hour MA to Trade at $63.69  FXDailyReport.Com

    Continue Reading

  • Fiji is Healthy to Visit Again as Dengue Outbreak Ends — Vax-Before-Travel

    Fiji is Healthy to Visit Again as Dengue Outbreak Ends — Vax-Before-Travel

    (Vax-Before-Travel News)

    Throughout 2025, many countries in the Pacific Region reported dengue fever outbreaks that affected thousands of people and disrupted vacation plans.

    A popular travel destination has announced some positive news.

    The Republic of Fiji Ministry of Health and Medical Services recently announced the end of the dengue outbreak in the Western and Central Divisions.

    As of August 8, 2025, Fiji’s government made this decision after concluding that the number of reported cases had decreased.

    The outbreak officially began in February 2025 in the Western Division and in April in the Central Division.

    About 11,599 dengue cases were reported across Fiji this year.

    While the outbreak has ended, mosquito-transmitted dengue viruses remain endemic in Fiji and a health threat to everyone.

    From a prevention perspective, numerious countries are offering a second-generation dengue vaccine that has demonstrated adequate protection against some of dengue’s four virus types.

    As of August 23, 2025, dengue vaccines are unavailable in the United States, except in Puerto Rico.

    Continue Reading

  • Google dialer: Why your android’s call settings have suddenly changed

    Google dialer: Why your android’s call settings have suddenly changed

    Many Android users are waking up to a new call and dialer screen without hitting the update button themselves. The reason? Google’s Material 3 Expressive Redesign is quietly rolling out to the Phone app. While the update promises a modern look and cleaner navigation, the surprise change is sparking debate across social media. Some call it sleek and fresh, while others argue it’s oversized, distracting, and unnecessary.

    What exactly has changed?

    The Phone app now wears a completely new face. Favourites and Recents no longer sit apart; they’re merged into a Home tab that shows both call history and top contacts in one carousel-like view.The Keypad has been pushed into its own separate tab, with rounder buttons replacing the older floating dialer. A redesigned Contacts menu hides behind a navigation drawer linked to the search bar.Even the incoming call screen works differently: a call can now be accepted or rejected through a horizontal swipe, similar to iOS, or toggled back to the classic single-tap setting. And once on a call, the buttons expand into pill-shaped icons, with the bright red End Call button demanding attention in a way that’s hard to ignore.

    Phone, Apps, and Tools Orbit

    Why is this bothering people?

    The problem isn’t change, it’s the way it arrived. Many users claim they never manually updated, yet woke up to a different dialer. Familiar buttons now look oversized and oddly blocky. The once-minimal design has been replaced with bolder shapes and heavier visuals.Social media is filled with reactions ranging from frustration to outright disbelief. One user wrote: “The phone app used to be perfection. Now the buttons are blocky, oversized, and ugly!” Another chimed in: “What in the HUGE MESS IS THIS?! I’m not blind bruh!!”The big red End Call button is the biggest eyesore for critics, too loud, too big, and too distracting for what used to be a subtle interface.

    Can it be changed back?

    Not entirely, but partially. The swipe-to-answer feature can be reverted to the old single-tap style through Settings > Incoming Call Gestures. However, the rest of the redesign has no quick toggle to roll back. Some users are resorting to uninstalling the latest Phone app update to restore the older design, though this isn’t a sustainable option for everyone.For now, the redesign seems here to stay. Unless Google offers a built-in “classic view” mode, users may have no choice but to adapt.For some, the new look feels cleaner. For others, it feels like an unwanted experiment dropped into daily life without warning. Either way, one thing is certain: the Phone app, once taken for granted, is suddenly the star of heated conversations online.


    Continue Reading

  • NASA’s Psyche captures images of Earth and the Moon from 180 million miles away

    NASA’s Psyche captures images of Earth and the Moon from 180 million miles away

    The Blue Marble (Image source: NASA’s Earth Observatory; edited)

    The Psyche spacecraft took a beyond-eagle-eye look back home. From 180 million miles (290 million kilometers) away, the spacecraft’s imager was able to capture Earth and its moon.

    Since its launch in October 2023, NASA’s Psyche spacecraft has been on course for asteroid Psyche. From what we know, Asteroid Psyche is likely made of a mixture of rock and metal — quite similar to Earth’s makeup. Hence, by studying this asteroid, scientists hope to understand how planets like Earth formed.

    But for Psyche’s mission to be successful, testing and calibrating its imager instrument is crucial. Given that asteroid Psyche reflects the Sun’s light, Psyche had to try to capture an object that also reflects the light from the Sun. Psyche had earlier taken images of Jupiter and Mars, both of which are redder than Earth.

    On July 20 and 23, Psyche calibrated its imager instrument by capturing Earth and its moon. The spacecraft’s imager comprises twin cameras. These cameras have filters and telescopic lenses that can take images of distant objects in different spectra of light.

    The team will continue testing the imagers. Saturn or Vesta is the likely target for the next test. The much bigger milestone for the Psyche spacecraft, though, is its flyby of Mars next year. Mars’ gravity will act as a slingshot to give the spacecraft the boost it needs to be able to arrive at asteroid Psyche in 2029.

    Image of the earth and moon as taken with Psyche (Image source: NASA, JPL-Caltech, and ASU)
    Image of the earth and moon as taken with Psyche (Image source: NASA, JPL-Caltech, and ASU)

    NASA: 1 and 2

    Image source: NASA (1) and 2 (linked above)

    Continue Reading

  • Neural correlates of span capacity during visual discrimination under varying cognitive demands

    Neural correlates of span capacity during visual discrimination under varying cognitive demands

    Participants

    We recruited 36 young participants from southern Taiwan through advertisements on the Internet and bulletin boards. We conducted a power analysis (G*Power 3.1.9.712, power = 0.95, effect size f = 0.25, α = 0.05, within-between subjects’ design, correlation among repeat measures = 0.5). The analysis indicated that a sample size of 36 participants would be sufficient to detect an estimated medium effect size. All participants were right-handed and without evidence of neurological or psychiatric disorders based on self-reports. This study was reviewed and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee at National Cheng Kung University (NCKU), Tainan, Taiwan, R.O.C., authorized by the Ministry of Education, Taiwan. All experimental procedures and the informed consent were obtained from all the participants and were approved under Approval No. NCKU HREC-E-112-120-2. All research was performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations, including the Declaration of Helsinki. Upon completion of all experiments — including computerized span tasks conducted outside the MRI scanner and a visual discrimination task performed inside the MRI scanner — participants received compensation of 1,500 New Taiwan Dollars (NTD). Detailed demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1.

    Span tasks outside an MRI scanner

    We assessed participants’ span capacity (individual differences) using computerized span tasks developed by Stone and Towse (2015) in JAVA13. The tasks included three complex span tasks and corresponding simple span tasks in both the verbal and visuo-spatial domains.

    Verbal domain

    Operation span task (complex span)

    The Operation Span task was chosen as the complex span task for the verbal domain (see Supplementary Figure S1). This task involved a repetitive sequence of memory and processing components. In each trial, participants were presented with an integer to memorize and recall in its original serial position at the end of the trial. Following each memory element (the integer), there was a processing phase where participants encountered a mathematical operation, such as ‘9 + 9 = 27’. They had to determine whether the presented answer was correct. Digits and operations were generated randomly for each trial, with digits ranging from 1 to 99. Each operation had an equal 50% chance of being correct, and the types of operations (multiplication, division, addition, subtraction) each had a 25% probability, ensuring a diverse range of operation types requiring both correct and incorrect responses. Digit span (simple span). Digit span corresponded to the simple span task for Operation Span. Essentially, it was Operation Span without the processing phase. Participants only needed to remember the digits and recall them in sequence at the end of the trial.

    Visuo-spatial domain

    Symmetry span task (complex span)

    The Symmetry Span task is a type of visuo-spatial complex span task where participants were required to recall grid locations in a 4 × 4 grid in the correct serial order (see Supplementary Figure S2). Following the presentation of each To-Be-Remembered (TBR) grid, participants engaged in a processing operation where they judged whether the presented pattern was symmetrical along the vertical axis, using the left/right arrow keys. Patterns were displayed on an 8 × 8 grid for this assessment. The recall phase began once the required number of storage-processing elements had been completed for a trial. Participants were prompted to recall by presenting them with the 4 × 4 grid, allowing them to click on the boxes in the order they remembered seeing them. Upon selection, a box turned blue, helping participants keep track of their responses. Matrix span (simple span). The matrix span task served as the simple span counterpart to the symmetry span task. Its procedure closely mirrored that of the symmetry span task, except for omitting the processing element.

    Rotation span task (complex span)

    The Rotation Span task was another visuo-spatial complex span task (see Supplementary Figure S3). The To-Be-Remembered (TBR) stimuli consisted of arrows characterized by two features: length (long or short) and angle of rotation (0°, 45°, 90°, 135°, 180°, 225°, 270°, or 315°). Participants were tasked with remembering these arrows presented in their correct serial order during the storage phase. In this complex span task, the processing operation involved presenting participants with a letter (F, G, or R) that could appear in its standard form or as a mirror image, and it could also be rotated at one of the 45-degree angles. Participants had to mentally rotate the image to determine whether the letter was presented normally or as a mirror image, using the left/right keys for their judgment. During the recall phase, participants were shown a 2 × 8 grid displaying all 16 possible arrows. The top row displayed all the long arrows, while the bottom row displayed all the short arrows. Participants used the mouse to select the arrows they recalled seeing in the correct sequential order. Arrow span (simple span). The processing phase was omitted in the arrow span task, which served as the memory span equivalent to the rotation span task. Consequently, the arrow span task focused on the participant’s ability to remember the arrows in their correct serial positions.

    Visual discrimination tasks with difficulty manipulation in an MRI scanner

    Participants engaged in a visual discrimination task featuring four distinct conditions, as outlined in Fig. 1. The task was programmed using OpenSesame14. Inside the MRI scanner, the stimulus display was projected onto a mirror affixed to the head coil. The task design was adapted from difficulty manipulations reported by Crittenden and Duncan (2014). These tasks varied in cognitive demands, ranging from simple perceptual discrimination to complex rule-based tasks11.

    Each trial began with a uniform gray screen in the baseline 4-line (4 L) condition. Participants were required to select the one odd line out of four based on length, considered the baseline or least demanding condition involving simple perceptual discrimination. At the start of each trial, a small fixation cross appeared at the center of the screen for 200 ms. Subsequently, four vertical lines were briefly presented (100 ms), aligned along the middle of the screen with their midpoints distributed symmetrically on either side of the fixation cross (total width 8.3° visual angle). Among these lines, three were equal in length (13.4°), while the fourth was consistently 50% shorter (6.7°). Participants indicated the position of the shorter line by pressing the corresponding key on an 8-button response box (e.g., Fig. 1, leftmost line shortest, response with the left middle finger). Responses were recorded only within the time window of the fixation cross, which persisted for 1000 ms after the lines disappeared from the display. Following a response, there was a jittered interstimulus interval of 500 to 1500 ms before the onset of the subsequent trial.

    The remaining conditions exhibited similarities with some alterations as described below. In the 8 L condition, similar to the 4 L condition but with eight lines instead of four, two additional vertical lines were presented on either side of the original four lines (total display width 16.7°; see Fig. 1). This required participants to use each hand’s little and ring fingers for a response. The 8 L condition increased the perceptual load by doubling the number of lines, thereby raising the cognitive demand compared to the 4 L condition.

    In the fine discrimination (FD) condition, similar to the 4 L condition, there were still four lines, but the shortest line was reduced to only 10% shorter than the other three lines (see Fig. 1). Participants had to discriminate between lines of very similar lengths, necessitating fine perceptual judgments. This condition heightened the cognitive demand further by requiring precise discrimination of line lengths, adding to the perceptual load.

    In the mapping switch (MS) condition, the stimulus-response mapping was modified from the natural one to the alternative illustrated in Fig. 1. Participants were still required to discriminate between lines of very similar lengths, making fine perceptual judgments. This condition was considered the most demanding, as participants had to select the right-most odd line but press the left-most button. This complex rule introduced higher-order cognitive processing involving working memory and response inhibition. It placed significant demands on executive functions and was generally regarded as the most challenging15,16. Participants practiced this condition until their accuracy rate reached 70% or higher.

    Trials were grouped into blocks, each dedicated to one task condition. Each block displayed a schematic similar to Fig. 1 indicating the upcoming condition in the middle of the screen for 2,000 ms. Following the cue, there was a 3,800 ms pause before the onset of the first trial. Each block consisted of 8 trials, with a total duration of 18,400 milliseconds. There was a 10-second interval between blocks. To maintain task engagement, the accuracy of each block was displayed at the end.

    The experiment was divided into three scanning sessions, each separated by a 30-second break. Within each session, there were 20 task blocks, comprising five blocks for each of the four conditions (4 L, 8 L, FD, and MS). The sequence of blocks was arranged in a pseudorandom order.

    Fig. 1

    Task conditions: In each scenario, participants were required to respond to the position of the shorter line using either four (conditions 4 L, FD, and MS) or eight (condition 8 L) alternative response buttons. Icons below each example display indicate the correct response for that display. The conditions are as follows: (a) 4 lines (4 L) condition, where participants respond to the position of the shorter line among four lines; (b) 8 lines (8 L) condition, where participants respond to the position of the shorter line among eight lines; (c) fine discrimination (FD) condition, where participants make a more precise response to the shorter line among four lines with smaller differences in length; and (d) mapping switch (MS) condition, where participants respond to the shorter line among four lines with reversed stimulus-response mapping. Arrows show the correct key presses for each target stimulus (shorter line) position.

    Behavioral data analysis

    Span task performance

    For each span task, we calculated their Full-Trial Accuracy (FTA) score. In the case of simple span tasks, points were awarded only when all Target-to-Be-Remembered (TBR) stimuli within a trial were correctly recalled. The score for each trial was determined by the loading of that particular trial, and the sum of scores across all trials constituted the FTA score for that task. In the case of complex span tasks, the calculation method was similar to that of simple span tasks, with the distinction that points were awarded only for trials where the response to the processing component was correct.

    Grouping participants based on a median split of the FTA score

    Our study employed the complex span task total scores to perform a median split, grouping participants into high-span and low-span groups based on individual differences in span task performances. This decision was based on the higher complex span scores compared to simple span scores, making them more effective for distinguishing individual differences in span capacity. However, we acknowledge that this approach may inadvertently capture differences in perceptual abilities, as evidenced by significant differences in simple span performance between groups (p <.005; see Results). Complex span tasks, which involve both storage and processing components, are known to be more sensitive measures of working memory capacity compared to simple span tasks that only require storage17. Given that our participant sample consisted of young adults, the complex span tasks were particularly effective in capturing individual differences in working memory capacity, which is critical for examining PFC activation patterns under varying cognitive demands18,19.

    Additionally, using the median split method allowed for a clear and balanced division of participants into two groups, facilitating the analysis of how these differences associate with neural activation patterns during task performance20. Specifically, using a median split ensured an equal distribution of participants into high- and low-span groups, facilitating balanced and well-built statistical comparisons. This approach also mitigated potential biases that could arise from uneven group sizes, enhancing the validity and reliability of our findings21. By employing a median split on complex span scores, we aimed to provide a clear delineation of how varying levels of individual differences impact neural activation patterns and cognitive control processes. However, we acknowledge that treating individual differences as a continuous covariate in a regression analysis might offer greater statistical power and a more profound understanding of its relationship with PFC activation. To address this, we conducted an additional regression analysis using complex span FTA scores as a continuous covariate, confirming that the observed patterns of PFC activation remained significant (p <.001; see Figure S4 in Supplementary Information).

    Visual discrimination task performance

    Behavioral performance (reaction time [RT] and accuracy) was measured separately for each of the four conditions (4 L, 8 L, FD, MS). Subsequently, we conducted a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the four conditions to determine if the behavioral performance replicated previous findings reported by Duncan and colleagues11. To test the hypothesis regarding whether individual differences would be associated with primary contrasts between conditions of interest in task performance, we initially compared the 8 L, FD, and MS conditions with the 4 L condition to identify any increase in reaction time (RT) and/or decrease in accuracy resulting from manipulation difficulty. Tasks with higher task demands, such as increased perceptual load in the 8 L condition, finer discrimination in the FD condition, or complex rule mapping in the MS condition, require greater working memory resources. High-span individuals typically exhibit superior cognitive control in these contexts22. In contrast, low-span individuals exhibited broader neural recruitment, which may reflect the additional engagement of brain regions under increased task demands23.

    Subsequently, we contrasted the conditions of interest. We used the 4 L condition as a baseline to evaluate how the 8 L, FD, and MS conditions demanded greater performance costs. The three contrast pairs were as follows:

    8 L–4 L (Perceptual Load): The primary difference lies in the number of items to be processed. The 8 L condition has double the items of the 4 L condition, increasing perceptual load and cognitive demand.

    FD-4 L (Precision of Discrimination): While both conditions involve selecting an odd line, the FD condition requires finer perceptual judgments, thereby increasing cognitive load compared to the broader discrimination required in the 4 L condition.

    MS-4 L (Complexity of Rule Application): The MS condition introduces a complex rule that reverses the typical response mapping, significantly increasing cognitive demands compared to the straightforward perceptual discrimination in the 4 L condition.

    We then employed mixed-design repeated-measures 2 (high- and low-span groups) x 3 (paired-contrasts: 8 L–4 L, FD-4 L, MS-4 L) ANOVAs on RT and accuracy, respectively, to examine the effects of the three contrasted conditions and determine if they interacted across two groups of individuals with high versus low span performances. Following the initial statistical testing, post hoc analyses were conducted using the Holm correction to further explore and compare the significant differences identified among the conditions.

    Imaging acquisition and analysis for the visual discrimination task

    The imaging data was gathered utilizing a General Electric (GE) Discovery MR750 3 Tesla scanner (General Electric Medical Systems, Milwaukee, USA) equipped with a 32-channel receive-only phased-array head coil at the Mind Research Imaging Center, National Cheng Kung University. High-resolution structural images were obtained using a fast-SPGR sequence comprising 166 axial slices (TR/TE/flip angle 7.6 ms/3.3 ms/12°; field of view (FOV) 22.4 × 22.4 cm2; matrix size 224 × 224; slice thickness 1 mm). Functional EPI images were acquired through an interleaved T2* weighted gradient-echo planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence (TR/TE/flip angle, 2000 ms/30 ms/78°; matrix size, 64 × 64; FOV, 22 × 22 cm2; slice thickness, 3 mm; voxel size, 3.4375 × 3.4375 × 3 mm). Each run comprised 368 volumes, with the initial eight being dummy scans discarded to mitigate T1 equilibrium effects.

    fMRI imaging preprocessing

    Functional imaging data were analyzed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL)24 software. The analysis process comprised several specific steps at the 1 st level: Initially, preprocessing involved correcting head motion artifacts using the Motion Correction FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool (MCFLIRT)25,26,27. Subsequently, the brain extraction tool eliminated non-brain tissue from the preprocessed MR images (BET27. The FSL Motion Outliers tool25,27 accessible at https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSLMotionOutliers, was then employed to detect outlier volumes based on frame displacement between volumes (exceeding the 75th percentile + 1.5 times the interquartile range). The results of this process were utilized to reduce the influence of those volumes in subsequent analyses. Individual brain functional images underwent registration to the high-resolution T1 structural image via linear transformation, followed by registration of the individual structural image to the standard MNI152 template via linear transformation28. The first-level General Linear Model (GLM) in FEAT tool29,30,31 was then established, incorporating a 9 mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel for spatial smoothing.

    Statistical analysis: fMRI blocked analyses for the visual discrimination task

    A general linear model (GLM) was used to estimate parameter values reflecting the mean difference between experimental conditions of the visual discrimination task. Contrasts were performed to identify the regions recruited more for the 8 L, FD, and MS conditions relative to the 4 L condition.

    The start time of each condition block’s stimulus to the endpoint of the block was captured and used to generate the onset file. The onset files for the four conditions were incorporated into the model as EVs (explanatory variables) and convolved with the double gamma hemodynamic response function. The six head motion parameters and the motion outlier data obtained in the previous step were included as covariates in the model for control.

    The second-level analysis integrated data from the three runs, and the results of the three paired-contrasts (8 L–4 L, FD-4 L, MS-4 L) obtained at the first level were averaged separately.

    To investigate our hypotheses regarding the high- and low-span groups, the subject-level files of the two groups were compared by averaging them separately according to the three paired-contrasts. We further examined the significant clusters for the combinations with simple main effects based on the ANOVA results of behavioral data, separately contrasting high span > low span and low span > high span.

    Whole-brain univariate analysis

    All group-level analyses involved computing the activation level across the whole brain region for each participant and submitting each of those to a group-level t-test, treating the participant as a random effect. We identified clusters of activity that were significant at a cluster-level rate of 0.01, using a 3.1 z-threshold to define contiguous clusters32. Subsequently, the estimated significance level of each cluster (derived from Gaussian Random Field theory) was compared with the probability threshold33. To account for potential confounding factors, weperformed a partial regression analysis to control for gender effects between the high- and low-span groups. This step aimed to isolate the specific contributions of individual differences observed in neural activation patterns between the two groups.

    fMRI preprocessing and GLM for multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA)

    The fMRI data were preprocessed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) 1234,35 implemented in MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA). The preprocessing steps included slice-time correction and realignment to correct head motion using a rigid-body transformation36. The T1 image was co-registered to the mean EPI image, and then both the T1 image and functional volumes were normalized to the MNI template. All images were resliced to a 2 × 2 × 2 mm voxel size, resulting in a data cube of 79 × 95 × 79 voxels. The onset files, marked with the start times for three runs and four conditions, were input into the GLM model. Head motion parameters obtained from realignment were included as regressors. After preprocessing, we obtained the beta maps and SPM.mat files for subsequent MVPA analysis.

    MVPA

    MVPA was conducted using the Decoding Toolbox (TDT, version 3.999 F) implemented in MATLAB, following standard preprocessing procedures. A whole-brain searchlight analysis was performed to identify regions whose activation patterns allowed classification between higher difficulty conditions and the 4 L condition used as the baseline37. The input data for the classifier were the beta maps obtained from preprocessing and GLM analysis, normalized to MNI standard space. For every voxel in the brain, a sphere with a radius of 5 voxels centered on that voxel was used to train and test a linear support vector machine (SVM) using leave-one-run-out cross-validation within each participant38,39. The classification accuracy of each sphere was assigned to the center voxel, resulting in a subject-level accuracy map. Accuracy maps were then entered into group-level analysis to identify regions where decoding accuracy was significantly above chance (50%). To ensure independence between training and testing phases, cross-validation was performed within each participant’s functional space. This method avoids assumptions of voxel-wise anatomical correspondence across participants38,40.

    Continue Reading

  • LHC bins PTI lawmaker’s plea against denotification by ECP

    LAHORE: The Lahore High Court’s Justice Khalid Ishaq on Saturday dismissed petitions filed by former opposition leader in the Punjab Assembly Malik Ahmad Khan Bhachar and former MNA Muhammad Ahmad Chatha, who challenged the Election Commission of Pakistan’s (ECP) notifications that de-notified them.

    The court observed that since the petitioners are fugitives from justice in every case, they cannot invoke this court’s jurisdiction for judicial review.

    The subtle but decisive distinction of a “nexus to the case” is the key factor that conclusively determines the applicability—or otherwise—of the fugitive disentitlement doctrine in a civil matter brought by a criminal fugitive.

    Applying this doctrine to the facts of these cases leads to the inescapable conclusion that the ECP’s notifications, challenged through these constitutional petitions, are inextricably linked to the petitioners’ convictions.

    The respondents questioned the maintainability of these petitions, arguing that the petitioners are convicts who have not submitted themselves to due process, remain at large, and have active perpetual arrest warrants. Therefore, they asserted, the petitioners are not entitled to invoke this court’s extraordinary constitutional jurisdiction under Article 199 of the Constitution.

    The Additional Attorney General for Pakistan argued this point of maintainability in reliance on various judgments. In brief, the Additional Attorney General contended that judicial review jurisdiction cannot be granted to the petitioners in these circumstances since equitable jurisdiction should not aid a fugitive from justice.

    He further argued that a citizen seeking this court’s intervention must first show how he is entitled to a remedy when he is guilty of flouting a judicial order—specifically, fleeing after conviction.

    Finally, he stated that the jurisdiction under Article 199 is extraordinary and equitable and should not be exercised by someone who has approached the court with “unclean hands,” being a fugitive from justice. The law officers representing the ECP largely adopted his arguments.

    Responding to the question of maintainability, the petitioners’ counsel argued that despite a criminal conviction, civil rights remain protected. For example, challenging the ECP’s impugned notifications via Article 199 is unrelated to the criminal conviction, and the fugitive status should only affect the specific cases in which conviction occurred—not all matters.

    It is worth mentioning that the petitioners had challenged their disqualification and sought to halt upcoming by-polls in their constituencies.

    The petitioners’ counsel argued that no proceedings can be initiated against a member of the assembly without a reference from the Speaker. They noted that the lawmakers were disqualified without being heard, which violates the principles of natural justice.

    The ECP disqualified the petitioners following their convictions in the May 9 cases by anti-terrorism courts. The trial court had sentenced each to 10 years’ imprisonment.

    Continue Reading

  • Kate Middleton breaks social media silence with special rugby message

    Kate Middleton breaks social media silence with special rugby message

    Kate Middleton breaks social media silence with special rugby message

    Kate Middleton sent a message of support to England’s women’s rugby team Friday ahead of their opening match against the United States at the Women’s Rugby World Cup.

    Princess Kate, patron of both the Rugby Football Union and Rugby Football League, posted on the official social media account she shares with Prince William, wishing the Red Roses “the very best as they kick off their Women’s Rugby World Cup campaign tonight.”

    “I look forward to cheering you on and seeing the team rise to the challenge on home soil!” the princess wrote, retweeting England’s pre-match message.

    Kate Middleton breaks social media silence with special rugby message

    England faces the U.S. at 7:30 p.m. at Sunderland’s Stadium of Light in their tournament opener, four years after losing to New Zealand in the previous World Cup final.

    Kate became patron of the rugby organizations in 2022, assuming roles previously held by Prince Harry. 

    She attended a training session at Twickenham after her appointment was announced.

    Rugby Football League chief executive Ralph Rimmer welcomed her patronage, saying the organization was “delighted” as England prepared to host multiple rugby league World Cups.

    Prince William serves as patron of the Welsh Rugby Union, while Princess Anne holds the same role for Scotland’s rugby union.


    Continue Reading

  • PM directs NDMA, district administration to continue rescue operations in Ghizer – RADIO PAKISTAN

    1. PM directs NDMA, district administration to continue rescue operations in Ghizer  RADIO PAKISTAN
    2. 200 people rescued after glacial burst in Gilgit-Baltistan’s Ghizer: Rescue 1122  Dawn
    3. Pakistan lake formed by mountain mudslide threatens ‘catastrophic’ floods  Reuters
    4. Shepherd’s alert saves entire village in G-B  The Express Tribune
    5. GB shepherd hailed as hero for warning that saved 200 lives in glacier burst  Geo.tv

    Continue Reading

  • If you’re over 60 and remember these 5 specific phone numbers, your memory beats 98% of your peers – VegOut

    If you’re over 60 and remember these 5 specific phone numbers, your memory beats 98% of your peers – VegOut

    Ask someone over 60 for their childhood phone number. Watch their eyes drift upward, their fingers twitch as if turning an invisible rotary dial. Then, like pulling a pristine file from deep storage, they’ll recite digits they haven’t dialed since Carter was president: “Madison 4-7829” or “BUtterfield 8-3000,” complete with the exchange name that marked you as Manhattan middle-class.

    This isn’t nostalgia; it’s archaeological evidence of a different cognitive economy. Before contacts lists and “Hey Siri, call Mom,” humans carried dozens of phone numbers in their heads like emergency supplies. The average person in 1975 knew 20-30 phone numbers by heart. Today’s average? Three, maybe four, and one of those is probably their own.

    But some minds held onto these numerical sequences like treasures, preserving not just the numbers but entire networks of connection from a pre-digital world. If you can still recite these five specific categories of phone numbers, you’re not just remembering digits—you’re carrying a vanished civilization in your hippocampus, proof that your memory has survived the great forgetting that technology enabled.

    1. Your childhood home phone number (complete with area code)

    This is the baseline test, the control group. Nearly everyone over 60 can recite their childhood phone number—it was the numerical version of your name, repeated thousands of times to friends, written on permission slips, given to anyone who might need to reach your parents.

    But here’s what separates exceptional memory from average: Can you remember it with the original area code, before the splits and overlays that carved up telephone geography? Can you recall when it changed from five digits to seven? Do you remember the exchange name—PLaza, KLondike, CRestview—that preceded the numbers?

    Those exchange names weren’t random; they were geographical markers that told you what neighborhood someone lived in. Madison meant the Upper East Side. BUtterfield meant the Upper West Side. Knowing someone’s exchange was knowing their social geography. If you remember not just the number but its cultural context—when calling after 9 PM was cheaper, when you had to wait for the party line to clear—your memory is preserving more than digits. It’s preserving an entire communication ecosystem.

    2. Your best friend’s family phone number from high school

    Your own childhood number was mandatory memory, but your best friend’s number from 1976? That’s the difference between standard-issue recall and exceptional retention. This number had zero practical value after graduation, no evolutionary reason to survive forty years of synaptic pruning.

    Yet there it is: 434-7892. Perfect. Complete. You remember the physical act of dialing it—the specific resistance of dragging the rotary dial to each number, the longer journey to 9, the quick snap back. You remember their mother’s voice: “Hello, Peterson residence.” The pause before asking, “Is Julie there?” The muffled shout: “Julie! Phone!”

    These numbers burned into memory because teenage friendships operate at emotional temperatures that turn experiences into permanent records. Every call was an event requiring courage and planning. Would her father answer? (Terrifying.) Would she be home? (Please, God.) The emotional encoding that accompanied these calls—anxiety, anticipation, joy—made them neurologically indestructible. They’re stored in the same vault as first cars and broken hearts: the permanence file.

    3. The local pizza place or Chinese restaurant number

    674-3100. That was Tony’s Pizza, and if you grew up within their delivery radius, that number is probably still lodged in your brain like shrapnel. No area code needed—this was hyperlocal knowledge, as specific to your neighborhood as the corner store that sold beer to minors.

    “Call Tony’s,” meant dinner was solved. No menu consultation, no Yelp reviews, no decision fatigue. Just seven digits to salvation. Some families had an entire portfolio memorized: Luigi’s for pizza (674-3100), Golden Dragon for Chinese (674-8899), Sam’s Deli for emergencies (674-2020). These weren’t written anywhere. They lived in the family’s collective memory like oral tradition.

    If you still know these numbers, you’re carrying commercial archaeology. Tony’s is now a Starbucks. Golden Dragon became luxury condos in 2003. But 674-3100 persists in your neural pathways, a ghost number for a ghost business in a ghost version of your neighborhood. These numbers are proof of when commerce was personal—when “Tony” actually answered at Tony’s, when the Chinese restaurant knew you meant “no MSG” without asking.

    4. Your grandmother’s (or other relative’s) phone number

    Long-distance was expensive. Calling Grandma in Cleveland from New York meant watching the clock, keeping it under three minutes, saving important news for Sunday evening when rates dropped. Her number was memorized not for convenience but for emergencies—and for the ritual Sunday call that connected extended families across America.

    These numbers often had quirks that made them memorable. Repeated digits (848-4848), patterns (234-5678), or numbers that spelled something on the phone pad. Your brain found ways to encode them: “Grandma’s number has three 7s in a row” or “Uncle Pete’s ends in 1-2-3-4.”

    The exceptional memory isn’t just knowing Grandma’s number—it’s remembering the whole constellation of extended family phone numbers. Aunt Marie in Phoenix: 602-555-7829. Cousin Bobby in Boston: 617-555-3456. These numbers mapped your family across America, each area code a different branch of the family tree. Social memory like this served an evolutionary purpose—keeping track of your tribe—which is why it persists when other memories fade.

    5. A phone number from your first job

    This is the memory separator, the number that divides exceptional recall from merely good. Remembering where you worked at 17 makes sense. Remembering the phone number? That’s different wiring entirely.

    438-7000. Henderson Insurance, where you filed papers the summer before college. Or 291-8888, the McDonald’s where you worked register junior year. These numbers had no emotional weight, no long-term utility. Yet somehow they survived the great forgetting that claimed so much else.

    Often, it’s because you had to recite them constantly. “Thank you for calling Burger King on Route 9, 291-8888, may I take your order?” Said 200 times per shift, eight shifts a week, for two summers. The repetition carved grooves so deep that forty years later, you can still hear yourself saying it in your teenage voice, with the precise intonation your manager demanded.

    Or maybe it was the number you gave to that girl from the mall, trying to sound important with your “work line.” The number you called from a payphone to say you’d be late. The number that represented your first taste of adult responsibility, encoded forever in the part of your brain that remembers beginnings. 

    The numbers that vanished

    What’s equally telling is what we don’t remember anymore. The video store. The movie theater’s showtimes hotline. The local weather number (often something memorable like 936-1212). Time and temperature (usually sponsored by a bank). These services that once required phone calls have been replaced by apps and websites, their numbers evaporating from collective memory.

    The doctor’s office, the dentist, the pharmacy—once memorized, now just entries in our phones. We’ve outsourced our memory to devices, trusting the cloud more than our own minds. The result is a kind of numerical amnesia, where most people under 40 can’t even recall their partner’s phone number without checking their phone.

    Final thoughts

    If you remember all five categories, you’re not displaying exceptional memory—you’re demonstrating a different architecture of mind. Before we outsourced our memories to silicon, we built internal databases that were remarkably robust. These numbers have survived decades, career changes, moves across country, the deaths of the people they connected to. They’re more durable than most marriages.

    This isn’t about generational superiority. It’s about neural adaptation. The same kids who couldn’t program a VCR could store thirty phone numbers effortlessly. Today’s kids can navigate six social media platforms simultaneously but can’t remember their mother’s cell. Each generation develops the cognitive tools their technology demands.

    But here’s what’s haunting: these remembered numbers are orphans now. You can still recite your grandmother’s phone number perfectly, but she died in 1987. Tony’s Pizza exists only in your neurons. Your best friend from high school? You haven’t spoken in thirty years, but their family’s phone number remains, waiting for a rotary phone that will never ring again. These numbers are the last witnesses to a world where memory was mandatory, where forgetting someone’s phone number meant losing them entirely. In preserving these digits, you’re preserving proof that human memory, when it had to be, was miraculous.

    What’s Your Plant-Powered Archetype?

    Ever wonder what your everyday habits say about your deeper purpose—and how they ripple out to impact the planet?

    This 90-second quiz reveals the plant-powered role you’re here to play, and the tiny shift that makes it even more powerful.

    12 fun questions. Instant results. Surprisingly accurate.

     


    Continue Reading