Blog

  • Got $1,000? 2 Cryptocurrencies to Buy and Hold for Decades

    Got $1,000? 2 Cryptocurrencies to Buy and Hold for Decades

    Many cryptocurrencies skyrocketed during the buying frenzy for speculative investments in 2020 and 2021. That rally was fueled by near-zero interest rates, stimulus checks, social media buzz, and commission-free trading platforms. But in 2022 and 2023, many of those tokens crashed as interest rates rose and a new crypto winter began.

    Over the past year and a half, investors have gradually pivoted back toward cryptocurrencies as interest rates declined and President Donald Trump’s crypto-friendly administration took the helm. So if you’re still bullish on cryptocurrencies, it might be a great time to go shopping again.

    Image source: Getty Images.

    You shouldn’t stake your life savings in cryptocurrencies, but it might be smart to set aside a modest $1,000 in a few tokens that could soar over the next few decades. I’d personally stick with the two largest cryptocurrencies — Bitcoin (CRYPTO: BTC) and Ethereum (CRYPTO: ETH) — instead of the smaller and more speculative meme coins.

    Bitcoin, the world’s most valuable cryptocurrency, still has plenty of upside potential for a few simple reasons. First, it’s still mined with an energy-intensive proof-of-work (PoW) consensus mechanism, which becomes more costly every four years with each “halving” that cuts its mining rewards in half. Its maximum supply is also capped at 21 million tokens. Nearly 19.9 million of those Bitcoins have already been mined, and the final token is expected to be mined in 2140. There isn’t much room for long-term inflation in this model.

    Bitcoin’s increasingly difficult mining process, scarcity, and deflationary nature make it more comparable to gold, silver, and other physical assets than many other cryptocurrencies. That makes it a potential hedge against inflation and the devaluation of fiat currencies.

    Bitcoin’s first spot price exchange-traded funds (ETFs), which were approved in January 2024, made it easier for retail and institutional investors to invest in the coin without a crypto wallet. Big companies like MicroStrategy (NASDAQ: MSTR) continued to accumulate Bitcoin, the Trump administration recently established a Strategic Bitcoin Reserve, and inflation-wracked countries like El Salvador and Central African Republic even adopted Bitcoin as a national currency for a while. All of those developments supported the notion that Bitcoin was becoming “digital gold.”

    Continue Reading

  • Paleontologists Unearth New Species of “Mystery” Dinosaur – SciTechDaily

    1. Paleontologists Unearth New Species of “Mystery” Dinosaur  SciTechDaily
    2. New dinosaur species unveiled at London’s Natural History Museum  Yahoo
    3. This Dinosaur Was the Speed Demon of the Jurassic Era—And It Was the Size of a Dog!  The Daily Galaxy
    4. Natural History Museum’s new dinosaur specimen acquired from London dealer David Aaron  Antiques Trade Gazette
    5. ‘New To Science’ Species Of Dinosaur Goes On Display At London’s Natural History Museum  Yahoo

    Continue Reading

  • Reuters X accounts ‘withheld’ in India: local media – World

    Reuters X accounts ‘withheld’ in India: local media – World

    The Reuters news agency’s X account handles were “withheld” in India “in response to a legal demand”, Indian outlet The Print reported on Sunday.

    The accounts for both Reuters and Reuters World were inaccessible in India; however, users were still able to access 30 other Reuters accounts, The Print added. The Reuters news website is also accessible.

    X users in India have posted screenshots showing that they are unable to access the news agency’s accounts.

    Reuters has yet to issue a statement, while the Indian Ministry of Electronics and Information Technology (MeitY) told India Today that no order was given to restrict the accounts.

    “There is no requirement from the Government of India to withhold the Reuters handle. We are continuously working with X to resolve the problem,” an official spokesperson for the ministry told outlet India Today.

    The Print reported that in previous instances of X restricting access to accounts in India due to legal requirements, the platform issued statements through its Global Government Affairs handle.

    The account has yet to issue a statement on the restriction.

    This is not the first time India has blocked accounts on the platform, formerly known as Twitter.

    According to an Instagram post by The Hindustan Times, the accounts were withheld as per orders issued amid Operation Sindoor, when India launched missiles at sites in Pakistan in May.

    “The centre (government) has reportedly responded to social media platform X blocking the account of news agency Reuters in India, saying that it is a mistake on the part of the Elon Musk-owned company,” the outlet wrote in the post’s caption.

    According to The Print, X issued a statement on May 8 saying it had “received executive orders from the Indian government” to “block over 8,000 accounts in India, subject to potential penalties including significant fines and imprisonment of the company’s local employees”.

    “In most cases, the Indian government has not specified which posts from an account have violated India’s local laws. For a significant number of accounts, we did not receive any evidence or justification to block the accounts,” X wrote.

    Though X stated that it disagreed with New Delhi’s demands, the platform withheld access to the account solely within India.

    Continue Reading

  • Neuroscientists detect decodable imagery signals in brains of people with aphantasia

    Neuroscientists detect decodable imagery signals in brains of people with aphantasia

    Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.


    A new brain imaging study published in Current Biology has uncovered surprising neural activity in people with aphantasia—a condition where individuals report being unable to form mental images. Although they describe a complete absence of visual imagery, their brains still show patterns of activity in the early visual cortex when they attempt to imagine visual stimuli. However, this activity differs in important ways from what’s seen in people who do experience vivid mental imagery, offering insight into how consciousness might be linked to sensory representations in the brain.

    Aphantasia is a relatively newly defined condition in which people are unable to form mental images voluntarily. While those with aphantasia can describe objects and scenes using words or concepts, they report no visual “pictures” in the mind’s eye. Since much of what is known about mental imagery comes from people who can generate vivid images, the researchers wanted to know what happens in the brain when someone with aphantasia tries to visualize something. Do they engage the same brain regions, or are there deeper differences in how their brains represent imagined information?

    To answer these questions, the research team compared people with aphantasia to individuals with typical visual imagery using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The goal was to examine how both groups activated early visual brain regions—especially the primary visual cortex—during attempts to visualize simple stimuli. The researchers focused on whether the brain could still represent specific content in people who lack a subjective visual experience.

    The study involved 14 participants with verified aphantasia and 18 control participants with typical imagery. All were right-handed and had normal or corrected vision. Participants completed the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire to assess their subjective imagery, and their imagery ability was further validated using an objective task called the binocular rivalry paradigm. This method measures how imagining a visual pattern affects what people perceive shortly afterward. As expected, those with aphantasia scored near the floor on the vividness questionnaire and showed little or no sensory bias in the binocular rivalry task, confirming that they lacked typical imagery experience.

    In the main experiment, the researchers used fMRI to measure brain activity while participants either viewed or attempted to imagine simple visual patterns—specifically colored Gabor patches—at specific locations on a screen. Each participant completed several types of scans: imagery generation, passive viewing, retinotopic mapping to define visual areas, and region-of-interest localization to pinpoint the parts of the brain involved in processing the stimuli. During the imagery task, participants received a visual cue indicating which pattern to imagine and where to place it in the visual field. After each attempt, they rated how vivid their imagery had felt.

    Although people with aphantasia gave extremely low vividness ratings—averaging around 1 on a 1-to-4 scale—their brain activity told a more complex story. In both groups, fMRI signals from early visual areas could be used to decode what kind of pattern a person was trying to imagine. In other words, the brain still encoded specific information about the content of the imagery—even in the absence of subjective experience.

    But there were clear differences in how that information was represented. In people with typical imagery, activity in the visual cortex showed expected patterns: stronger responses in the hemisphere opposite to the side of the visual field where the stimulus was imagined. In contrast, people with aphantasia showed the reverse: stronger responses in the same-side hemisphere (ipsilateral) instead of the opposite (contralateral). This suggests a different functional organization of visual activity during imagery attempts.

    While the imagery content could be decoded in both groups, only in the control group did the patterns of brain activity overlap between imagery and actual perception. In the control group, algorithms trained on imagery-related brain data could accurately identify visual stimuli seen during passive viewing—and vice versa. This kind of cross-decoding failed in the aphantasia group. Their visual cortex did encode information about imagery attempts, but those patterns did not match those generated during real visual perception.

    This mismatch might explain why people with aphantasia experience no visual imagery even though their brains generate structured representations during imagery tasks. According to the researchers, the results point to a difference not just in the strength of visual signals, but in their format. The activity in the visual cortex of people with aphantasia appears to be “less sensory,” meaning it may lack the specific qualities that give rise to conscious visual experience.

    The researchers also looked at broader brain networks. During imagery attempts, people with aphantasia showed stronger activity in brain regions associated with language and auditory processing, such as the superior temporal gyri. They also had weaker functional connections between these regions and visual areas. This could indicate that when people with aphantasia try to visualize, they may rely more on verbal or conceptual strategies rather than generating vivid internal images.

    To test whether differences in attention or effort might explain the results, the researchers ran a follow-up study with control participants. These individuals were asked to imagine either a clear or blurry version of the same visual patterns. Their reported effort levels and brain activation were similar across both conditions, suggesting that differences in subjective clarity do not necessarily reflect differences in cognitive effort. This makes it less likely that the patterns seen in aphantasia are simply due to lower motivation or task engagement.

    Although the findings shed new light on the neural basis of aphantasia, the authors note several limitations. The sample size was relatively small, especially given the rarity of aphantasia, and most participants in both groups were women. Also, while the study focused on low-level visual features, it did not examine whether similar results would hold for more complex images, such as faces or scenes. The absence of eye-tracking during scanning means researchers could not fully rule out whether subtle eye movements influenced the neural signals.

    But the results still offer evidence that people with aphantasia can generate structured, content-specific activity in the visual cortex, even though they lack a conscious image. This dissociation between brain activity and experience challenges long-held assumptions that activity in early visual areas is directly tied to visual awareness. Instead, it suggests that not all neural representations are created equal—some may carry enough sensory information to generate conscious images, while others may not.

    The study opens new avenues for understanding the neural basis of mental imagery and visual consciousness. Future research could explore what kinds of information are encoded in the brain during imagery attempts in aphantasia, and whether different feedback connections in the brain might account for the altered representations.

    The study, “Imageless imagery in aphantasia revealed by early visual cortex decoding,” was authored by Shuai Chang, Xinyu Zhang, Yangjianyi Cao, Joel Pearson, and Ming Meng.

    Continue Reading

  • Faith Kipyegon and Beatrice Chebet smash world records at an extraordinary Diamond League meeting

    Faith Kipyegon and Beatrice Chebet smash world records at an extraordinary Diamond League meeting



    CNN
     — 

    Kenya’s Faith Kipyegon and Beatrice Chebet both set new world records on Saturday at an extraordinary Diamond League meeting in Eugene, Oregon.

    Kipyegon, who fell short in her quest to become the first woman in history to run a four-minute mile last month, bounced back in impressive style to break her own women’s 1,500m world record with a time of 3:48.68.

    She dominated the race, holding off Australia’s Jessica Hull who stuck with her until the back straight, eventually winning by almost three seconds.

    As Kipyegon crossed the line, the crowd erupted, knowing she had shaved 0.36 seconds off the world record, while she turned and pointed back towards the clock before wrapping herself in a Kenyan flag.

    Earlier in the meet, also known as the Prefontaine Classic, Chebet obliterated the 5,000m world record by more than two seconds, becoming the first woman to complete the distance in under 14 minutes.

    She crossed the line in 13:58.06, more than three seconds ahead of Agnes Jebet Ngetich in second place, who recorded the third-fastest time in history.

    Gudaf Tsegay, the previous world record holder, finished the race in third place with a time of 14:04.41.

    Chebet had already broken the 14-minute barrier on the road in January when she completed the race in 13:54.

    “I’m so happy,” Chebet said afterward. “In Rome (where she recorded a time of 14:03.69), I was just running to win a race. After Rome, I say that I am capable of running a world record so let me go back home and prepare … I told myself, ‘if Faith is trying for a world record in Eugene, why not me too?’”

    “It’s a good track for me,” she added, referencing her previous success in Eugene where she broke the women’s 10,000m world record last year.

    Now, Chebet holds the 5,000m and 10,000m women’s world records as well as the Olympic titles in both events.

    Elsewhere at the meet, Mondo Duplantis comfortably won the men’s pole vault but failed to raise his own world record, hitting the bar on all three of his attempts to clear 6.29m.

    Continue Reading

  • This CEO went from folding clothes at Nordstrom to being Jeff Bezos’ right hand man—the billionaire told him to delegate to his employees more

    This CEO went from folding clothes at Nordstrom to being Jeff Bezos’ right hand man—the billionaire told him to delegate to his employees more

    During Greg Hart’s monotonous college job of folding t-shirts and jeans at Nordstrom, he never imagined that one day he’d be right-hand man to one of the most notable CEOs in the world—or even be an executive himself.

    But after landing a job at Amazon in the late 1990s—when it was a bookstore-focused startup—Hart started his steep climb up the corporate ladder. In 2009, he had worked his way up to Jeff Bezos’ technical advisor, a chief-of-staff-equivalent role internally known as “the shadow.”

    Now, as a new CEO of Coursera, Hart is taking the lessons he learned sitting next to the now third-richest man in the world (with a net worth of $241 billion) to transform the world of educational technology. That includes, he says, being someone who is always willing to be curious and to listen to your gut—even if the data might not back you up.

    “One of the things that Jeff would regularly say is when the data and the anecdotes don’t align, trust the anecdotes,” Hart tells Fortune. “Because it probably means that you’re either measuring the wrong thing in the data, or that the data is telling you something that you’re just not seeing yet.”

    This mantra would prove especially relevant in Hart’s life after he was tapped to lead Amazon’s creation of Alexa—a level of success he hopes to emulate at Coursera.

    “(Coursera) is one of the leaders in the edtech space, certainly,” Hart says. “But hasn’t yet achieved what I would call the true breakout success that I was fortunate to have seen when I was at Amazon.”

    Reshaping edtech with lessons learned from Amazon and Alexa

    While serving as technical advisor, Hart was asked to turn Bezos’ two-sentence idea for the implementation of virtual assistant technology into a product found in consumers’ homes, later known as Alexa. 

    With little background in hardware or software, Hart recalls being hesitant: “Why am I the right person to tackle this challenge?”

    “He (Bezos) was unbelievably gracious. He said, ‘you’ll do fine, you’ll figure it out,’” Hart says.

    Now with more than 600 million Alexa devices sold, it’s clear Hart did figure it out—in part thanks to the belief Bezos instilled in his subordinates. That lesson is one Hart says was critical in making Amazon grow from a bookstore to an e-commerce conglomerate.

    “Pushing decisions down as close to the customer as possible was certainly something that I learned from Jeff,” he says. “The fewer decisions that have to go to the CEO, the faster the organization will move.”

    Moving quickly will likely play in Coursera’s favor considering the rapidly changing world of education, including AI’s intersection with skill development. 

    One of Coursera’s biggest competitors, 2U (which also owns edX), filed for bankruptcy last year and became a private company. Coursera’s public performance hasn’t been spectacular, either. Its share price currently sits at about $8.50, a far cry from its 2022 IPO at about $45.

    But in the next five years, a predicted 1 billion people will gain internet access, according to Bloomberg, and thus would have access to Coursera’s thousands of online learning opportunities, that include partnerships with both industry and universities including Google, Microsoft, and IBM as well as Stanford University, University of Michigan, and University of Pennsylvania.

    “There is a huge opportunity for Coursera, not just to serve all the people who are online today and give them access to world class education so they can transform their lives, but also to do that for the population that will come online,” Hart says.

    Hart’s advice for Gen Z: Optimize for learning—and ask why

    For young people looking to push their careers up the corporate ladder and emulate a jump from Nordstrom to Amazon,Hart’s advice is simple: Focus on learning—not on a flashy job title or cushy salary.

    “Don’t optimize for titles, don’t optimize for salary, optimize for learning. And if you do that in the long run, it will benefit you,” he tells Fortune.

    And while that advice may sound on-brand coming from the CEO of an education company, treating your career like a marathon, not a sprint, and spending time discovering your broader interests is a mantra echoed by other business leaders, including Hart’s former coworker, Andy Jassy. 

    “I have a 21-year-old son and a 24-year-old daughter, and one of the things I see with them and their peers is they all feel like they have to know what they want to do for their life at that age,” Jassy, the current Amazon CEO, said on the podcast, How Leaders Lead with David Novak. “And I really don’t believe that’s true.”

    “I tried a lot of things, and I think that early on it’s just as important to learn what you don’t want to do as what you want to do, because it actually helps you figure out what you want to do,” Jassy added.

    And while Jassy admits career success involves an element of luck and may include multiple setbacks, remaining persistent is what ultimately might land you a shot at the corner office. 

    “I feel like my journey or adventure was a lot of luck, and I think maybe one of the things I did best was not overthink it,” Jassy added to Novak.

    Continue Reading

  • Exynos 2500 trades blows with the Snapdragon 8 Elite in GPU benchmark but lags behind in CPU performance

    Exynos 2500 trades blows with the Snapdragon 8 Elite in GPU benchmark but lags behind in CPU performance

    The Galaxy Z Flip7 looks set to deliver identical CPU performance to the Galaxy Z Flip6. (Image Source: Samsung)

    The Samsung Galaxy Z Flip7 has now made several trips to Geekbench ahead of its official debut at Unpacked. The foldable impresses in the OpenCL test but only performs as well as the last-gen Snapdragon 8 Gen 3 on the CPU side.

    A few weeks ago, Samsung quietly unveiled the Exynos 2500 as its flagship chipset for the year. While the Exynos 2500 isn’t widely expected to keep up with its peers in the CPU department, it appears things may be different on the GPU side.

    As shared by Abhishek Yadav, the Exynos 2500 on Samsung’s upcoming Galaxy Z Flip7 records a score of 18,601 on Geekbench’s OpenCL test. That’s a respectable score, as Geekbench’s official chart lists the Snapdragon 8 Elite for Galaxy-powered Samsung Galaxy S25 Ultra with 18,365 on the same test. 

    While this is just one benchmark—and, as such, not definitive evidence of the chipset’s performance—it does seem like the Exynos 2500’s RDNA 3-based Xclipse 950 GPU will be competent at the very least. That isn’t particularly surprising, as the Exynos 2400 outpaced the A17 Pro and Snapdragon 8 Gen 3 on a several GPU benchmarks as well. 

    Things are less impressive on the CPU side, unfortunately. The Galaxy Z Flip7 in its “SM-F766B” guise has made multiple trips to Geekbench and mostly underperformed. The last of those visits saw the foldable score 2,313 and 7,965 on the single-core and multi-core tests respectively, with both of those scores indicating CPU performance merely on par with last year’s Snapdragon 8 Gen 3. 

    The Exynos 2500 only matches the Snapdragon 8 Gen 3 in CPU tests.
    The Exynos 2500 only matches the Snapdragon 8 Gen 3 in CPU tests.
    The Exynos 2500 looks set to offer competitive GPU performance.
    The Exynos 2500 looks set to offer competitive GPU performance.

    Continue Reading

  • NEOC issues alert of severe rainfall, potential flooding – RADIO PAKISTAN

    1. NEOC issues alert of severe rainfall, potential flooding  RADIO PAKISTAN
    2. NDMA issues monsoon alert, warns of flood risk across Pakistan until July 10  Ptv.com.pk
    3. Floods devastate Dadhocha villages  The Express Tribune
    4. Heavy rain, flooding likely from today  Dawn
    5. Karachi receives new rain spell  ARY News

    Continue Reading

  • Azma Bukhari says religious unity prevails across Pakistan on Ashura

    Azma Bukhari says religious unity prevails across Pakistan on Ashura

    Punjab Information Minister Azma Bukhari has reaffirmed the presence of religious harmony across the country on the day of Ashura, urging citizens to shun those inciting sectarianism.

    Speaking to media in Faisalabad, she said Ashura symbolizes the spirit of sacrifice and stressed that maintaining peace is a shared responsibility.

    She outlined that the Punjab government has enforced comprehensive security arrangements for Muharram processions and gatherings.

    For the first time, a cyber patrolling force has been deployed during Muharram to monitor online activity, Bukhari said, adding that the entire provincial administration is actively managing Ashura-related events.

    She emphasized public safety and service delivery as key government priorities, citing improved sanitation, installation of cold water stations along procession routes, and inspection visits in various districts. She also noted receiving goodwill messages for the Chief Minister during her visits.

    The minister revealed that objectionable and sectarian content on social media is being strictly monitored, with several arrests made and 417 hate-spreading websites blocked so far.

    Bukhari concluded that the government’s actions are not symbolic, but practical efforts to uphold peace, calling on the public to show mutual respect and reject divisive forces.


    Continue Reading

  • Ancient Neanderthal ‘Fat Factory’ Reveals How Advanced They Really Were : ScienceAlert

    Ancient Neanderthal ‘Fat Factory’ Reveals How Advanced They Really Were : ScienceAlert

    The Neanderthals are our closest extinct relatives, and they continue to fascinate as we peer back through tens of thousands of years of history.

    In a new discovery about this mysterious yet often familiar species, researchers have found ancient evidence of a Neanderthal “fat factory” in what is now Germany.

    Operational around 125,000 years ago, the factory would’ve been a place where Neanderthals broke and crushed the bones of large mammals to extract valuable bone marrow and grease, used as a valuable extra food source.

    Related: Neanderthal DNA Exists in Humans, But One Piece Is Mysteriously Missing

    According to scientists, this is the earliest evidence yet for this type of sophisticated, large-scale bone processing, including both bone marrow and grease: the first confirmation Neanderthals were also doing this some 100,000 years before our species made it to Europe.

    “This was intensive, organised, and strategic,” says archaeologist Lutz Kindler from the MONREPOS Archaeological Research Center in Germany.

    “Neanderthals were clearly managing resources with precision – planning hunts, transporting carcasses, and rendering fat in a task-specific area. They understood both the nutritional value of fat and how to access it efficiently – most likely involving caching carcass parts at places in the landscape for later transport to and use at the grease rendering site.”

    The researchers found their evidence on a site called Neumark-Nord in eastern Germany, not far from the city of Halle. They uncovered more than 100,000 bone fragments from what are thought to be at least 172 large mammals, including horses and deer.

    Researchers at work at the Neumark-Nord site. (Kindler et al., Science Advances, 2025)

    A good proportion of the bones showed cut marks and signs of intentional breakage, pointing to deliberate butchering – these weren’t just leftovers from a hunt. There were also indications of tool use and fires in the same location, all in a relatively small area.

    Add all of that together, and it seems clear that some kind of systematic, organized bone processing was going on here. Similar processes have been linked to Neanderthal sites before, but not at this level of scale or sophistication.

    “Bone grease production requires a certain volume of bones to make this labour-intensive processing worthwhile and hence the more bones assembled, the more profitable it becomes,” says archaeologist Sabine Gaudzinski-Windheuser from MONREPOS.

    We can add this to the long list of studies that have revealed Neanderthals were much smarter than they’re often made out to be. Thanks to recent research we know they were adept swimmers, capable brewers, and abstract thinkers – who raised their kids and used speech patterns in a similar way to humans.

    Ultimately though, Homo sapiens thrived and survived, while Neanderthals died out. That’s another story that archaeologists are busy investigating the whys and wherefores of, but all we have of the Neanderthals now are the remains and the sites they left behind – which will no doubt give up more revelations in the future.

    “The sheer size and extraordinary preservation of the Neumark-Nord site complex gives us a unique chance to study how Neanderthals impacted their environment, both animal and plant life,” says computer scientist Fulco Scherjon from MONREPOS.

    “That’s incredibly rare for a site this old – and it opens exciting new possibilities for future research.”

    The research has been published in Science Advances.

    Continue Reading